Cognitive style
Cognitive
style are typically discussed
as if they were polarities, in reality, humans more likely show a tendency
towards one pole or the other, with their scores on cognitive style tests
arranged along a continuum between the poles. We use the term ‘tendency
‘advisedly. As with the state versus ‘trait’ distinction for personality
variables, Witkin and Goodenough (1981). There is the question of the
uniqueness of each cognitive style and indeed, whether certain cognitive styles are distinct from
general intelligence (Roach 1985). Perhaps the cognitive style that has received the most
attention in the SLA literature, starting with Naiman etal. (1978), who did
find a link between it and SL achivement, is field independence and dependent.
1)
Field
independent/ dependence
People are termed field dependence if they are unable to
abstract an element from its context, or be ground field in support of their hypothesis,
Naiman et al. found
that field independent twelfth grade student scored higher on imitation and
listening comprehension task than did subject who were field dependentt.
Tucker, Hamayan and
Genesse (1976) also reported that a trait factor which included field independence significantly
predicted the France scores of Anglophone seventh-grade students on a standardizes achievement test.
Thus, most of the
available evidence offers support for relationship betweenen field independence
and the second language learning success. Brown suggests that field
independence may be important to the classroom learning and performance on
paper-and-pencil test; however, when it comes to untutored SLA will be determined
be by how well the earner can communicate with speakers of the TL, and empathy
will help in this regard .it is interesting to note that of their three measures
of language proficiency.
Some researchers have suggested that the tendency to field
independence or dependence may be culture bound. Frad and scarpaci (1981) found
that the student from Latin American countries more field-dependent than their
non latin counterparts. By the contrast, Hansen (1984) did find the cultural
differences for this cognitive style. Hansen studied 286 subjects between the
ages of fifteen and nineteen in six pacific island cultures. She found that
Hawaiian subject were more field independent than samoan, Hansen’s finding
provides some evidence for Cohen’s (1969) hypothesis that the more analytic
style develops i highly industrial and technological societies, whereas field dependent
is more typical of agrarian
societies.
2. Category width.
The cognitive style of
the category width refers to certain people’s
tendency to
include many items in one
category. Category width is often
measured by pettigrew’s width scales (1958),
which consists of twenty multiple choice
items for which subject are asked to estimate some variable based on the
information given. H.D brown (1973) and schumann (1978) have hypothesized that
broad categorizers may formulate more rules than are necessary to account for
TL phenomena.
3.
Reflectivity/impulsivity.
Individuals who have a
reflective cognitive style tend to mull thing over when making a decision.
According to HD. Browm (1980) the matching
familiar figures test (MFFT) (kagan et el. 1964) is most often used to measure
reflectivity impulsive.
Subjects are presented with a figure and then a number of facsimiles. Subject’s
response time in making a match a match is considered a measure of this
cognitive style. Subject who tak are considered longer, but make a fewer errors,
are considered reflective ; those with the opposite pattern are considered
impulsive.
4.
Aural/visual.
This cognitive style
refers to a person’s preferred mode presentation: aural or visual. Levin at al. (1974) observe that
many learners could be considered bimodal, i.e. learning via one mode or the
other does not contribute appreciably to a difference in outcome. But for a
sizeable minority, approximately 25 percent of all learners, the mode of
instruction clearly do influence their success as learners.
lepke (1977), reporting
on a study of university students in the US learning German, claimed that when
the student were taught through their preferred modality, they performed better
. lepke (1977), France student at a junior college in Texas not only performed
better when they had a choice of modality presentation, but also there was a substantial
increase in enrolment in language courses over what there had been when
student’s preference did not determine the modality of instruction.
5. Analytic/ gestalt
Peters
(1977) has demonstrated that children approach the SL learning task i different
way. Some children seem to take language word by word, analysing it into component;
others approach language in a more holistic or Gestalt-like manner. Peter’s
subject Min, a young Vietnamese boy, was an example of learner with Gestalt
style. Minh learned characteristic intonation contours for TL phrase before he
learned to reader all the segmental. To be sure, Minh would articulate certain
words in phrases or
sentence, but he also used ‘filler syllables’ between those
words as place-holders so that he could preserve the correct intonation contour.
Peters portrays his performance as Minh’s learning the tune before the words.
Ventriglia (1982) makes a three way distinction among beaders, brainders and
orchestrators. Beaders are he
analytic learners who learn the meaning of each word and then string them
together to making meaning. Braiders
are more holistic in their approach in that they assimilate language in chunks
and more daring about using the in social context than thair more cautious
counterpart, the beaders. Orchestrators, like beaders, are likely to be slow to
start producing the language but use sounds rather than words as the building
blocks.
Hemisphere specialization
In
our earlier discussion of age related factor in SLA, we introduced the notion
of lateralization. Lateralization is a process whereby each of the two hemispheres
of the brain becomes increasingly specialized. Evidence for this
differentiation comes from studying the behavior of patients who have had their
left or right hemisphere. In all right handed individuals and approximately
two-thirds of the left handed individuals, the left hemisphere specializes in
logical. The right hemisphere is specialized for spatial relations and for task
which involve matching some part of a schema to a whole. In fact, image be they visual, tactile or
auditory are perceived and remembered by subjects using their right hemisphere
even when they find the image hard to describe of name special talents of the
left hemisphere (hartnett 1975).
How
these observations relate to SLA is certain individuals
perform relatively better on test Using one hemisphere or the other and thus
are thought to be left or right hemisphere dominant. If this is the case, it
might offer neurophysiologic basis for those individuals may be hemi left hemisphere
dominant. Learners who are more field dependent and holistic in their approach
may then be more right hemisphere dominantt. indeed, evidence at
least for one of these associations comes from Cohen, Barent and Silverman
(1973) cited in hartnet 19850. Although not exactly to relate to individual
defferentces, there is nonetheless a body of neurolinguistic research which has
been devoted to exploring the role of the right hemisphere in bilinguals. It
stand to reason that if there is increasing lateralization as the brain
matures, the way in which the hemisphere enter into the L1 And L2 process would
differ. In particular since it is generally agreed that the language centre of
the brain is the left hemisphere researcher have sought to identify the role of
the right hemisphere in SLA (Selinger 1982; Genesee1982).
Ventiglia(1982)
makes a three way distinction among beaders braiders and orchestrators. Beiders
are the analytic learners who learn the meaning. Braiderers are
the analytic learners
who learn the meaning of each word and then string them together to make
meaning. Braiders are more holistic in their approach in that they assimilate
language in chunks and then string them in social context than their more cautious
counterparts, the beaders. Orchestrators do not process of TL neither by word
nor by chunk, rather they attend to the sound pattern of the TL, paying
particular attention to the meaning to the meaning of the intonation contours
initially.
No comments:
Post a Comment